Warning: mkdir(): Permission denied in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 81 Warning: fopen(/home/virtual/audiology/journal/upload/ip_log/ip_log_2024-04.txt): failed to open stream: No such file or directory in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 83 Warning: fwrite() expects parameter 1 to be resource, boolean given in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 84 Substitution Patterns of Phoneme Errors in Hearing Aid and Cochlear Implant Users

Substitution Patterns of Phoneme Errors in Hearing Aid and Cochlear Implant Users

Article information

J Audiol Otol. 2017;21(1):28-32
Publication date (electronic) : 2017 March 30
doi : https://doi.org/10.7874/jao.2017.21.1.28
1Division of Speech Pathology and Audiology, Research Institute of Audiology and Speech Pathology, College of Natural Science, Hallym University, Chuncheon, Korea.
2Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology, Hallym University Graduate School, Chuncheon, Korea.
Address for correspondence: Woojae Han, PhD. Division of Speech Pathology and Audiology, Hallym University, 1 Hallimdaehak-gil, Chuncheon 24252, Korea. Tel +82-33-248-2216, Fax +82-33-256-3420, woojaehan@hallym.ac.kr
Received 2016 May 11; Revised 2016 June 01; Accepted 2016 June 09.

Abstract

Background and Objectives

It is acknowledged that speech perceptual errors are increased in listeners who have sensorineural hearing loss as noise increases. However, there is a lack of detailed information for their error pattern. The purpose of the present study was to analyze substitution patterns of phoneme errors in Korean hearing aid (HA) and cochlear implant (CI) users who are postlingually deafened adults.

Subjects and Methods

In quiet and under two noise conditions, the phoneme errors of twenty HA and fourteen CI users were measured by using monosyllabic words, and a substitution pattern was analyzed in terms of manner of articulation.

Results

The results showed that both groups had a high percentage of nasal and plosive substitutions regardless of background conditions.

Conclusions

This finding will provide vital information for understanding the speech perception of hearing-impaired listeners and for improving their ability to communicate when applied to auditory training.

Introduction

In general, speech perception is assessed clinically through two methods. One method is to measure a listener's correct percentage percentage of a listener's correct response by offering speech materials under various conditions such as noise [1], reverberation [2], time alteration [3], and frequency filtering [4]. The other method of assessment is to estimate incorrect answers or errors of speech perception and to analyze their patterns [5]. When conducted with monosyllabic words, for example, the error patterns of hearing aid (HA) and cochlear implant (CI) users were examined under quiet [6] and noise conditions [7]. The researchers found that the error patterns of substitution and no response showed much higher numbers than the other patterns, thus suggesting that auditory training could be applied to reduce the highest number of errors first of all-at first. In other words, to find the most common error pattern in which hearing-impaired listeners had trouble and to reduce it with the highest priority might give us valid data for creating an effective approach to auditory training. However, little attention has been paid to a practical link between error patterns and auditory training. Furthermore, we need to study more detailed information about the error pattern of the hearing-impaired listeners who wear the HA and CI as analyzing a specific subcategory of the highest error pattern in order to devise a plan for efficient auditory training. To foster this goal, the purpose of the present study was to analyze the error patterns of phonemes for hearing-impaired listeners in quiet and noisy conditions and to gauge the phoneme having high errors, which should be an initial step to apply in auditory training, resulting in improvement of the speech perception ability of impaired listeners.

Subjects and Methods

Subjects

Thirty-four hearing-impaired listeners participated in this study: 20 HA and 14 CI users. The mean age in the HA and CI groups was 52.5 years [standard deviation (SD): 14.90] and 44.5 years (SD: 15.46), respectively. Etiologies of the individual subjects varied, but they all had symmetrical bilateral sensorineural hearing loss. The duration of auditory deprivation was 13.32 years (SD: 11.64) in the HA group and 17.77 years (SD: 13.87) in the CI group. Experience with HA and CI was 5.19 years (SD: 5.07) and 2.51 years (SD: 0.36), respectively, in those groups. Seventeen out of 20 HA users and three CI users were wearing the device bilaterally.

Fig. 1 indicates the unaided and aided hearing thresholds of HA and CI groups in terms of the better ear, although they had symmetrical bilateral hearing loss. There was a significant difference in the unaided threshold in all frequencies between the two groups. However, in the aided threshold, the CI group showed slightly better detection in high frequency regions than the HA group, as expected. However, the most comfortable levels (MCL) of the two groups showed little difference bilaterally, indicating 58.5±3.67 dB HL and 57.14±2.26 dB HL for HA and CI, respectively, in the aided condition. All subjects were native Korean speakers and all completed the informed consent form before the experiment.

Fig. 1

Group mean of hearing threshold in dB HL, a function of frequency in hearing aid (HA) and cochlear implant (CI) groups

Test procedure

Before initiating the experiment, all participants were checked for normal middle-ear status in an otoscopy examination and were confirmed to have a type-A tympanogram (Madsen Zordiac 901; GN Otometrics, Taastrup, Denmark). They were also tested for pure-tone audiometry by air and bone conductions from 250 to 8,000 Hz (TympStar v2; Grason-Stadler, Eden Prairie, MN, USA) using a headphone (TDH 39; Telephonics Co., Farmingdale, NY, USA) for the unaided condition and a speaker for the aided condition. As stimuli, the Korean monosyllabic test [8] consisted of 50 words per list and was produced by one male speaker recorded on a compact disc speaking on a compact disc player (4 lists×50 words=total 200 words). The stimuli were presented binaurally in quiet and at two different signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs), i.e., +6, 0 dB with white noise through the speaker (0° degree, 1 meter) in a sound isolation chamber. The presentation level of the monosyllables was initially set to each subject's MCL and then adjusted so that the syllables were equally loud independent of SNRs. For an easy listening condition, the quiet condition was tested first and then +6, 0 dB SNRs followed. Among four lists of the monosyllable test, three lists (e.g., one list per listening condition) were randomly selected and tested across the subjects. When the subject listened to each monosyllable, he/she produced it with accuracy. Based on recording responses of the participant, two testers wrote down the errors in accuracy if the participants responded incorrectly, and they determined the errors as a final answer after discussing discrepancy in testers' opinions.

Data analysis

First, the error scores of both groups were calculated as a function of noise level. That is, the total number of errors for each condition was counted by one subject and was classified into seven error patterns: substitution, addition, omission, substitution plus omission, substitution plus addition, fail, and no response. Second, among seven error patterns, a substitution that was the most dominant error pattern was recalculated to be maximized as a percentage at each condition. Finally, the substitution pattern was analyzed and classified into the manner of articulation such as nasal, affricative, fricative, liquid, and plosive based on a suggestion in a study by Healy, et al. [9].

Results

As expected, the total number of errors per subject gradually increased from 13.2 to 37 for the HA group and from 21.5 to 40.36 for the CI group as noise increased (Table 1). The HA users had a lower total error number than the CI users. Most error patterns of the two groups showed higher error numbers at the higher noise levels, except for the omission pattern of the CI group and a pattern of the substitution plus omission in the HA group. The error number of addition pattern did not change much in the HA group, whereas that of the CI group was the lowest in 0 dB SNR. The error number of the no response pattern increased dramatically for both HA and CI groups as noise increased. Remarkably, the substitution error pattern had the highest number in the six error patterns for both HA and CI users regardless of background conditions, indicating that substitution was the most robust error of the Korean hearing-impaired listeners (see the results of substitution in Table 1).

The average error number per subject for total and seven patterns

The substitution patterns of phoneme error for HA and CI groups in quiet, +6, and 0 dB SNR are classified by five types in the manner of articulation. The pie graphs of Fig. 2 used the error number of the substitution pattern in Table 1 to be maximized as a percentage. Each value in the pie graphs represents the substitution error percentage for a specific type. For example, the HA group had approximately 43% error of nasal substitution in all three conditions, while plosive and fricatives substitution patterns followed as 38.55% and 10.24% for quiet, 37.46%, and 13.38% for +6 dB SNR, and 38.11% and 12.38% for 0 dB SNR, respectively. The nasal substitution of the CI group also stood out conspicuously, having 37.42%, 43.18%, and 41% error for each condition. Similar to the HA group, the CI group also showed plosive and fricative substitution patterns as the following pattern. This explained that the most dominant error patterns of substitution were nasal and plosives in both groups. When the nasal and plosive substitution errors were added, the HA and CI groups showed them as high as 81.32% and 70.97%, 80.6% and 78.03%, and 80.78% and 80.00%, respectively, for quiet, +6, 0 dB SNR.

Fig. 2

Graphical comparison of substitution error pattern for hearing aid (HA, top row), and cochlear implant (CI, bottom row) groups as a function of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR).

Discussion

The present study was designed to analyze patterns of the phoneme error in HA and CI users under quiet and noise conditions. Regardless of the noise levels, the substitution pattern showed the highest error and the nasal and plosive substitutions were remarkable in the error patterns of those users. These results should explain why there is a large substitution pattern in the hearing-impaired listeners. Ching, et al. [10] argued that after wearing the assistive listening devices, the hearing-impaired listeners are able to hear speech that they would otherwise miss hear or not hear at all. However, the impaired listeners often perceive that speech incorrectly and in a different manner to normal-hearing listeners because of speech distortion caused by amplification and noise. This distortion might differ based on degree of hearing loss and period of auditory deprivation [11], yet consequently result in specific confusions or substitution errors by the HA and CI users as their characteristics [121314] although substitution pattern of phoneme errors is changed to a fail pattern or no response pattern under a great amount of background noise [7] and there is a large individual variance [6]. Regardless, this pattern of hearing-impaired listeners was supported by our results in that the combination of nasal and plosive substitutions seemed to be consistent in either quiet or noise conditions.

According to results from Healy, et al, [9] the hearing-impaired listeners gained large benefits in the manner of articulation whereas listeners with normal hearing generally experienced the greatest gains in terms of the place of articulation. In particular, Korean nasals /n, m, ŋ/, which are formed by blocking the oral passage and allowing the air to escape freely through the nose, have common acoustic features, namely “nasal formant” and “anti-formant” [15]. The nasal formant or so-called nasal murmur has a high intensity and low first formant at 250-300 Hz. Thus, the nasals have limitations in helping hearing-impaired listeners perceive speech and can create confusion with other sounds [9] even though the murmur provides important information for the perception as transition cues. The anti-formant property of the nasals also provides much weak energy in the second and third formants of frequency range between 300 and 4,000 Hz with large bandwidth, when compared to acoustical energy of other sounds. For example, the nasal cavity has a large area and absorbs more sound in the walls of the vocal tract, resulting in increased damping and lower amplitude of spectral peak. Furthermore, to perceive a cue of the second formant transition, which is a dynamic cue and is used when changing from consonant to vowel, is very difficult for hearing-impaired listeners who lack language experience and thus cannot receive a main cue under low-redundancy speech [16]. On the other hand, it is acknowledged that there is an adverse relationship between hearing loss and plosive perception. Although burst feature is the primary acoustic feature for consonant identification, it is very sensitive in noise [17]. Thus, if a listener has hearing loss, the Korean plosives will cause great confusion to him under noisy conditions, as we confirmed in our study.

There are two limitations of the present study that warrant further research. First, we need to determine whether vowel context has any systematic effect on consonant recognition for proving our current data in terms of the robust nasal substitution. A study by Donaldson and Kreft [18] showed that consonant-recognition scores varied by 15% or more depending on following various vowels. They concluded that vowel-context effects were strongest for /d, j, n, k, m, l/ consonants, and two of them are nasal phoneme. A second issue comes from our findings, i.e., nasal or plosive substitution of speech recognition in the hearing-impaired listeners, whether speech production characteristic in hearing-impaired listeners, is nasality. Previous studies have shown that excessive nasality is often observed in the speech of hearing-impaired listeners who cannot adequately monitor this subtle characteristic of speech [19]. That is, simultaneous oral and nasal airflows were obtained from a group of moderate-to-profound hearing-impaired adults during production of the stop consonants /p, t, k, b, d, g/ [20].

Despite these limitations, the results of the current study have implications for speech amplification strategy in noise and aural rehabilitation for hearing-impaired listeners. For one possible practical application, clinicians should focus on discrimination and identification of nasal and plosive substitutions. For instance, if nasal substitution is dominant in a HA user, auditory training for reducing nasal substitutions would be an initial step for him to enhance his speech perception ability. Because this strategy is able to reduce the highest error with accuracy at the beginning of auditory training, it will provide effective training protocol for either clinicians or patients in terms of a shorter total training period and a saving of expenses.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2015S1A3A2046760).

Notes

Conflicts of interest: The authors have no financial conflicts of interest.

References

1. Armstrong M, Pegg P, James C, Blamey P. Speech perception in noise with implant and hearing aid. Am J Otol 1997;18(6 Suppl):S140–S141. 9391635.
2. Helfer KS, Wilber LA. Hearing loss, aging, and speech perception in reverberation and noise. J Speech Hear Res 1990;33:149–155. 2314073.
3. Gordon-Salant S, Fitzgibbons PJ. Sources of age-related recognition difficulty for time-compressed speech. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2001;44:709–719. 11521766.
4. Nittrouer S, Tarr E, Wucinich T, Moberly AC, Lowenstein JH. Measuring the effects of spectral smearing and enhancement on speech recognition in noise for adults and children. J Acoust Soc Am 2015;137:2004–2014. 25920851.
5. Gordon-Salant S. Phoneme feature perception in noise by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired subjects. J Speech Hear Res 1985;28:87–95. 3982002.
6. Lee JH, Kim JH. Comparison of word and environmental sound recognition by cochlear implant and hearing aid users. Audiol Speech Res 2011;7:28–39.
7. Chun H, Ma S, Han W, Chun Y. Error patterns analysis of hearing aid and cochlear implant users as a function of noise. J Audiol Otol 2015;19:144–153. 26771013.
8. Kim JS, Lim DW, Hong HN, Shin HY, Lee KD, Hong BN, et al. Development of Korean standard monosyllabic word lists. Audiol Speech Res 2008;4:126–140.
9. Healy EW, Yoho SE, Wang Y, Apoux F, Wang D. Speech-cue transmission by an algorithm to increase consonant recognition in noise for hearing-impaired listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 2014;136:3325. 25480077.
10. Ching TY, Dillon H, Katsch R, Byrne D. Maximizing effective audibility in hearing aid fitting. Ear Hear 2001;22:212–224. 11409857.
11. Cox RM, Alexander GC. The abbreviated profile of hearing aid benefit. Ear Hear 1995;16:176–186. 7789669.
12. Gordon-Salant S. Consonant recognition and confusion patterns among elderly hearing-impaired subjects. Ear Hear 1987;8:270–276. 3678640.
13. Kates JM. On using coherence to measure distortion in hearing aids. J Acoust Soc Am 1992;91(4 Pt 1):2236–2244. 1597612.
14. Phatak SA, Yoon YS, Gooler DM, Allen JB. Consonant recognition loss in hearing impaired listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 2009;126:2683–2694. 19894845.
15. Kim YS. An acoustic, aerodynamic and perceptual investigation of word-initial denasalization in Korean London: Doctoral thesis, UCL (University College London); 2011.
16. Nittrouer S, Thuente Burton L. The role of early language experience in the development of speech perception and language processing abilities in children with hearing loss. Volta Rev 2001;103:5–37.
17. Kapoor A, Allen JB. Perceptual effects of plosive feature modification. J Acoust Soc Am 2012;131:478–491. 22280609.
18. Donaldson GS, Kreft HA. Effects of vowel context on the recognition of initial and medial consonants by cochlear implant users. Ear Hear 2006;27:658–677. 17086077.
19. Chen MY. Acoustic parameters of nasalized vowels in hearing-impaired and normal-hearing speakers. J Acoust Soc Am 1995;98(5 Pt 1):2443–2453. 7593928.
20. Lock RB, Seaver EJ 3rd. Nasality and velopharyngeal function in five hearing impaired adults. J Commun Disord 1984;17:47–64. 6715568.

Article information Continued

Fig. 1

Group mean of hearing threshold in dB HL, a function of frequency in hearing aid (HA) and cochlear implant (CI) groups

Fig. 2

Graphical comparison of substitution error pattern for hearing aid (HA, top row), and cochlear implant (CI, bottom row) groups as a function of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR).

Table 1.

The average error number per subject for total and seven patterns

Hearing aid
Cochlear implant
Quiet +6 dB 0 dB Quiet +6 dB 0 dB
Number of total error 13.2 25.55 37 21.5 36.64 40.36
Type of error pattern
 Substitution 8.3 14.95 15.35 11.07 18.86 14.28
 Addition 0.75 0.7 0.75 1.93 1.21 0.5
 Omission 0.35 0.4 0.9 0.86 0.28 0.57
 Substitution plus omission 0.2 1.4 0.7 0.71 1.28 2
 Substitution plus addition 0.7 1.65 2.95 0.93 3.21 3.43
 Fail 2.25 3.8 4.8 4.71 7.86 8.43
 No response 0.65 2.65 11.55 1.28 3.93 11.14