A target signal that setting in another masking sound can be better detected by adding energy that is remote in frequency from both the masker and target signal [
1]. This effect is known as comodulation masking release (CMR) and is observed when the remote sound and the masker share coherent patterns of amplitude modulations [
1-
6]. CMR is measured in two ways. The first (earlier method) often referred to as the “band-widening experiment,” is to use a single band of noise, centered around the signal frequency, as a masker, and to compare thresholds for modulated and unmodulated noise maskers as a function of the masker bandwidth. The second method (preferential method) is to use a masker consisting of several narrow masker bands of noises, typically with bandwidths between 20 and 50 Hz, which have relatively slow inherent amplitude modulation; one band is centered at the signal frequency (on-frequency masker) and one or more other bands (flanking bands or off-frequency maskers) are spectrally separated from the signal frequency [
2].
CMR has been investigated in many studies and most of them used the second method. In most of them, the arrangement of the flankers is considered symmetric and asymmetric arrangement of flankers is less used. Two reviews that were somewhat similar to ours, related to Yasin, et al. [
7] and Dau, et al. [
1]. Yasin, et al. [
7] examined CMR in two arrangement; with and without flankers. Their results showed no significant difference in masking release results between in two situation, existence or absence of flankers (there was on-frequency masker). In the review of Dau, et al. [
1], after the high-frequency flankers, a multi-flanker was added as postcursor and CMR measured in two arrangements. Their results also showed no significant difference in two arrangements. In Dau, et al’s study [
1] which was an interesting study, the symmetrical arrangement of flankers was also used and examined the effect of perceptual grouping mechanisms on CMR. The results are consistent with the idea that modulation analysis occurs within, not across, auditory objects, and that across-frequency CMR only occurs if the on-frequency and flanking bands fall within the same auditory object or stream [
7-
11]. In the evaluation of CMR with symmetric flankers at a high-frequency signal, there is a limitation in output of clinical audiometers for the high-frequency flankers. Because most audiometers are not capable to have frequency >12,000 Hz, therefore CMR evaluation at higher frequencies is not available and this explain why CMR has not been tested at the frequencies higher than 8-10 kHz. In the CMR usually the flankers are symmetrically in the up and down to the signal. For example in the CMR review at 3,000 Hz (as signal), the low-frequency flankers are 2,000 Hz and 1,000 Hz. While high-frequency flankers include 4,000 Hz and 6,000 Hz. There will be no problem in this regard. Now if we want to check the CMR at 8,000 Hz (as signal), there will be no problem in generation of low-frequency flankers (i.e., 4,000 Hz and 2,000 Hz or 6,000 Hz and 12,000 Hz). But the high-frequency flankers in this case will be 16,000 Hz and 32,000 Hz, which clinical audiometers can not product them. Considering the asymmetry of traveling wave in the cochlea at the higher frequencies (in the base of cochlea) and the lack of effect of them on the lower frequencies (in the apex of cochlea), the question is whether limiting the number of high-frequency flankers has any effect on CMR results or not? According to Fletcher theory, there are many filters in cochlea that each one had a shape. Considering to the shape of auditory filters and their position of them in cochlea (high in base and low in apex of the cochlea), we assumed that the symmetry or asymmetry of the flankers is not affected CMR results. In fact due to location and the asymmetric shape of auditory filters at high-frequency (base of cochlea) it seems that the high-frequency tone not affected the low-frequency tone (apex of cochlea) that is present at the same time. Critical band of filters specified with equivalent rectangular bandwidth (ERB) and ERB in low and highfrequency is completely difference. Therefore a high-frequency tone did not marked effect on low-frequency tone, because ERB in both of them did not has any overlapping [
12-
14]. Our main assumption in this study was that symmetry or asymmetry of flankers have no effect on CMR results. For that, we aimed in this study to measure CMR by using asymmetric flankers and comparing the results to the symmetric-flankers measured CMR.