Warning: mkdir(): Permission denied in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 87 Warning: chmod() expects exactly 2 parameters, 3 given in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 88 Warning: fopen(/home/virtual/audiology/journal/upload/ip_log/ip_log_2024-10.txt): failed to open stream: No such file or directory in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 95 Warning: fwrite() expects parameter 1 to be resource, boolean given in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 96 Efficacy of the Digit-in-Noise Test: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Efficacy of the Digit-in-Noise Test: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Article information

J Audiol Otol. 2022;26(1):10-21
Publication date (electronic) : 2021 November 16
doi : https://doi.org/10.7874/jao.2021.00416
1Division of Speech Pathology and Audiology, College of Natural Sciences, Hallym University, Chuncheon, Korea
2Laboratory of Hearing and Technology, Research Institute of Audiology and Speech Pathology, College of Natural Sciences, Hallym University, Chuncheon, Korea
3Department of Otolaryngology-Head & Neck Surgery, College of Medicine, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Korea
4Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
Address for correspondence Woojae Han, PhD Division of Speech Pathology and Audiology, College of Natural Sciences, Hallym University, 1 Hallymdaehak-gil, Chuncheon 24252, Korea Tel +82-33-248-2216 Fax +82-33-256-3420 E-mail woojaehan@hallym.ac.kr
Received 2021 July 19; Revised 2021 August 16; Accepted 2021 September 4.

Abstract

Background and Objectives

Although the digit-in-noise (DIN) test is simple and quick, little is known about its key factors. This study explored the considerable components of the DIN test through a systematic review and meta-analysis.

Materials and Methods

After six electronic journal databases were screened, 14 studies were selected. For the meta-analysis, standardized mean difference was used to calculate effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals.

Results

The overall result of the meta-analysis showed an effect size of 2.224. In a subgroup analysis, the patient’s hearing status was found to have the highest effect size, meaning that the DIN test was significantly sensitive to screen for hearing loss. In terms of the length of the presenting digits, triple digits had lower speech recognition thresholds (SRTs) than single or pairs of digits. Among the types of background noise, speech-spectrum noise provided lower SRTs than multi-talker babbling. Regarding language variance, the DIN test showed better performance in the patient’s native language(s) than in other languages.

Conclusions

When uniformly developed and well validated, the DIN test can be a universal tool for hearing screening.

Introduction

Early screening of hearing loss is essential for all people with any experience of noise exposure or aging. In other words, a hearing test is the first step in the treatment of hearing loss [1]. Although a simple hearing test using pure tones has been developed and used [2], it is limited when identifying hearing problems in daily life because the test is usually conducted in an artificially quiet environment, not in a naturally noisy one. Moreover, a major complaint of people with hearing loss is that they can hear the speech, but do not understand it, especially in the presence of background noise [3]. Thus, testing the speech perception of people who are suspected of having a hearing loss should take place under noisy conditions.

As an alternative, many researchers have adopted the speechin-noise test that uses simple digits, called the digit-in-noise (DIN) test [3-17]. The DIN test can easily and reliably (self-) screen that a patient has hearing loss by using a simple presentation method with a single digit (e.g., 0, 1, 2) and/or a series of digits (e.g., 3-6-1). Unlike other elements of speech, such as syllables, words, and sentences, the DIN test is rarely affected by a patient’s auditory and cognitive ability [7,10]. As a result, the DIN test could be administered to even non-native speakers of a language [11]. Based on these advantages, the DIN test is suitable for hearing screening test to early detection of hearing loss and fitting of hearing assistive devices such as hearing aid and cochlear implant [7,12].

The DIN that has been developed by many researchers since 2000 is now available in Dutch, US English, UK English, Persian, Polish, Australian English, Canadian English and Canadian French, South African English, Flemish, French, Greek, German, Swedish, Swiss, Italian, Mandarin, Russian, and Spanish (Supplementary Fig. 1 in the online-only Data Supplement). It can also be used on telephones, smartphones and tablets in convenience. Also, the DIN test currently used as the hearing screening tool especially for the Netherland and South Africa [7,10]. Nevertheless, there are discrepancies in the contents and testing methods due to different perspectives among the researchers who developed the DIN. In this light, using systematic review and meta-analysis, this study examines the major factors to consider when developing and administering the DIN test: the patient’s hearing status, types of stimuli and noise, language comparisons, and the patient’s language competency.

Materials and Methods

Search strategy

All the processes, containing inclusion criteria, article search strategy, and article selection were followed by a Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) statement [18] and the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) of Cochrane Collaboration [19], both of which have been used for a systematic search and meta-analysis of the published articles.

The precise definition of inclusion and/or exclusion criteria is necessary to identify the homogeneity and reliability of the eligible studies. For the inclusion criteria of articles for this systematic review and meta-analysis, a strategy of participants, intervention, control, outcome measures, and study design (PICOS) was applied [18]. Table 1 displays the PICOS criteria used in this study. Animal studies, data papers, general articles (e.g., narrative reviews, conference abstracts, letters, books and book chapters, magazines, and conference proceedings), and articles not written in English were excluded.

Inclusion criteria for the current study based on participants, intervention, control, outcomes, and study designs (PICOS)

Article selection

Six electronic journal databases—Embase, MEDLINE, PubMed, Web of Science, Science Direct, and Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health—were used to search for the articles. Miller, et al. [20] had used digits as test material to identify any context effect for speech intelligibility and compared those materials to words or syllables. Since then, as an alternative material for speech and/or hearing screening, digits have been used in more recent studies [3-16]. Thus, all authors discussed the time frame of the article search and selection as January 1951 to December 2020. The key terms were “digit-in-noise test” AND “single digit” OR” digit pair” OR “digit triplet” AND “hearing screening test” AND “language” AND “background noise” AND “hearing loss” OR “normal hearing.” These terms were combined to minimize the need to filter out duplicate papers.

Fig. 1 depicts each step of the systematic article search and selection process. A total of 51,796 records were searched using six electronic journal databases. After eliminating 4,192 duplicates, 47,604 records remained. The titles and abstracts of 47,604 records were screened, resulting in the exclusion of 32,251 records. Then, the full texts of the remaining 15,353 records were reviewed at the eligibility stage. Finally, only 14 records met the PICOS criteria for this study, and they were included in the systematic review and meta-analysis.

Fig. 1.

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) flow diagram that visually expresses the inclusion and exclusion process of the current study. PICOS, participants, intervention, control, outcome measures, and study design.

Study quality and potential sources of study bias

To evaluate both the study quality and any potential sources of study bias, we used the 11-item Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro) Scale [21]. The scale assesses the quality of eligibility criteria, randomization and concealment of subjects, baseline of intervention, blinding of subjects and therapists, and key outcomes (Table 2). Each item was assigned 1 for “yes” or 0 for “no.” After evaluating the items, the quality of each study was ranked as “excellent” (9 to 11), “good” (6 to 8), “fair” (4 to 5), and “poor” (below 4). The findings of the highest-scoring studies were considered the most valid [22]. All authors conducted the study quality and potential sources of study bias process independently.

Analysis using the scientific study validity criteria based on PEDro checklists

The data contained in the articles were independently extracted and synthesized into six categories by the authors: 1) participants (number, age, and hearing threshold); 2) intervention (types of stimuli and noise, test condition, and language); 3) control group; 4) outcome measures; 5) study design; and 6) main findings.

Meta-analysis

The Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software (Ver. 3, Biostat Inc., Englewood, NJ, USA) was used for the meta-analysis. Fourteen articles were reviewed to determine whether their data were suitable, while utilizing the descriptive statistics (mean and standard deviation values in the experimental and control groups). Because the characteristics of the qualitatively synthesized data from the included articles were continuous and the types of outcome measures were different, standardized mean differences (SMDs) were used to calculate the effect sizes for each study. After calculating these effect sizes, a summary estimate was examined. The random-effect model was chosen to calculate both effect size and summary estimate. The funnel plot and Egger’s regression test were used to identify publication bias.

The Higgins I2-statistics and Cochran’s Q-test were used to confirm heterogeneity across the articles. The I2 values were expressed as the percentage from 0 to 100. For example, the interval ranges from 0 to 25%, 25% to 75%, and 75% to 100% of I2 value were considered as low, middle, and high heterogeneity, respectively [23]. The Q values for the Cochran test indicated a total variance across the dataset of the articles. This test showed statistical significance at 95% of confidence interval (CI), and heterogeneity across the dataset of articles.

However, since the articles were categorized based on outcome measures, subgroup analysis was conducted to compare the hearing condition, types of stimuli, types of noise, and a subject’s language competence. A meta-regression was applied based on three remarkable features (sound attenuation, sound localization, and speech perception) because of the possibility of high heterogeneity and/or different outcome measures for the subgroup analysis.

Results

Evaluation of study quality

The study quality calculated by the PEDro checklist showed a mean score of 6.64 (standard deviation [SD]: 1.15, range: 4-8 scores). Twelve of the 14 studies were ranked as “good,” with total values of 6 to 8 [3-4,6-8,10-16]. The remaining two studies [5,9] were evaluated as “fair,” with scores between 4 and 5.

Participants

The PICOS criteria of the reviewed articles are summarized in Table 3. In most of the articles, participants consisted either of adults with normal hearing [3,7,9,12-13,15] or adults with hearing loss [3-6,14,16]. Potgieter, et al.’s [10] study tested adults with both normal hearing and hearing loss. Interestingly, the studies by Jansen, et al. [8] and Smits, et al. [11] described their participants as “ears.” After recruiting the participants, their left and right ears were rated in terms of hearing thresholds (i.e., normal hearing and hearing loss).

Characteristics and main findings for all enrolled studies for the participants, the intervention, control group, and the outcome of each study

Most of the studies concentrated on young adults [7,9,12-13,15], old adults [3,14,16], middle-aged adults [6], both young and middle-aged adults [8], and both young and old adults [4-5,10]. Unfortunately, the study conducted by Smits, et al. [11] did not mention the age of the “ears.”

Intervention

The types of digits used for testing were analyzed as the intervention. Although most studies used triple digits as their stimuli, their composition differed slightly. For example, of the nine studies that used 10 digits from 0 to 9, five applied all 10 digits [9-10,12-13,15], and four studies used all digits except for 7 [5,14], 7 and 9 [11], and 0, 7, and 9 digits [4].

Three articles selected the digits 1 to 10 instead of 0 to 9. Ebrahimi, et al. [6] used 1 to 10; while Wilson, et al. [3] and Wilson and Weakely [16] used 1 to 10, but excluded 7. The remaining two articles used either 1 to 9, again with the exception of 7 [7] or without exception [8].

Controls

Of the 14 articles, half had a control group of adults with normal hearing. This group comprised young adults [3,5,9], young and middle-aged adults [16], and young and old adults [4,7]. However, Jansen et al.’s [8] control group mentioned only the total number of ears. The remaining seven articles conducted repeated measures with an additional purpose [6,10-15].

Outcomes

The reviewed studies were classified into three outcome measures. Although the outcomes reported in all articles were consistent in their speech recognition thresholds (SRTs), the expressions were slightly different: SRT [4,8-10,12-15], SRTn which uses SRT with a digit [5,7,11], signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) [3,6], and the point where a subject hit 50% correct scores [16].

Study design

Five of the studies used between-group comparisons [3,4,10,14,16]. All 14 studies provided repeated measures.

Overall results in meta-analysis

The results of effect size for the studies using the random-effect model are presented in Fig. 2A. To consider the characteristics of the dataset, the data with mean and SD was collected for using the SMDs methods.

Fig. 2.

Forest plot (A) and funnel plot (B) of the 14 reviewed studies analyzed using standardized mean differences.

The studies resulted in SMDs of 2.224 (95% CI: 1.371-3.077, p<0.001). The funnel plotting is displayed in Fig. 2B. Based on the results of Egger’s regression analysis, the results of the meta-analysis showed no publication bias (Intercept: 8.77, 95% CI: 5.390-12.154, p<0.001). The Higgins I2-statistics and Cochran’s Q-test showed that heterogeneity was high (I2 : 96.83%, Q: 63.012, p<0.001).

To identify the results of the meta-analysis more clearly, a subgroup analysis was conducted based on hearing status, types of noise and stimulus, and language comparison.

Subgroup analysis

Fig. 3 depicts the results of the subgroup analysis. The first subgroup was divided into two groups with or without hearing loss (Fig. 3A), resulting in the highest effect size of 3.754 (95% CI: 2.840-4.669). It confirmed that the SRT of individuals with normal hearing was lower (or better) than that of the adults with hearing loss and/or hearing loss simulated by using the frequency filters.

Fig. 3.

The forest plot for the subgroup analysis by hearing condition (A), types of stimulus (B) and noise (C), kinds of language (D), and language competence (E).

For stimulus types, the subgroup analysis consisted of studies that reported on the comparison between stimuli, such as single digits, paired digits, triple digits, and a sentence (Fig. 3B). Triple digits showed lower SRTs (the negative value of 50% SRT means a better noise threshold) compared to a single digit or sentence (effect size: 1.538, 95% CI: -0.952-4.029). In Fig. 3C, the noise type subgroup showed the effect size of 2.753 (95% CI: 0.654-4.852), indicating that speech-spectrum noise provided lower SRT than did the condition of multi-talker babble noise.

As the language variance was compared for the different languages, the effect size showed 2.008 (95% CI: 0.307-3.708) (Fig. 3D). This result demonstrated that the DIN test with subject’s own language had a significantly lower SRT than the DIN test with previously developed or applied to a more frequently used language such as English. However, the comparison between native and non-native speakers resulted in an effect size of -1.090 (95% CI: -1.412--0.768) for the SMDs method (Fig. 3E) while supporting the view that non-native subjects had a slightly lower SRT than the native subjects, in contrast to our expectation.

Discussion

The present study aimed to examine several important factors of the DIN test-hearing status, types of stimulus and background noises, and effects of language competency-by using a systematic review and meta-analysis techniques. The studies were screened and confirmed based on these inclusion and/or exclusion criteria. Fourteen studies were identified for a specific eligibility process of that review and quantitatively synthesized to conduct a meta-analysis.

When conducting the DIN tests, is it sensitive in difference between patients with and without hearing loss?

The hearing status of subjects was significantly affected by the results of the DIN test. In other words, this meta-analysis showed that individuals with hearing loss needed higher SNR to achieve similar performance than did adults without hearing loss. In fact, hearing loss reflects high hearing thresholds, creating difficulties with discriminating and understanding incoming speech sounds; patients therefore need a better hearing condition, such as higher SNRs. The correlation between pure-tone average (PTA) and SRT could be a way of demonstrating the consistency between two thresholds [11]. A positive and significant correlation between PTA and the DIN test was also found in the results of our subgroup analysis. These relationships of three thresholds are supported by Denys, et al. [4] who demonstrated a high correlation from Pearson’s r value of 0.66 to the 0.86 between PTA and SRT measured by the DIN test, while providing useful information to use when diagnosing a hearing loss.

Smits, et al. [11] found a strong positive correlation between a speech-in-noise test and the DIN test (r=0.866). This result was also confirmed by our present study. When considering the characteristics of the test material, however, the DIN test was less affected by contextual cues and linguistic factors [7,10] than the speech-in-noise test was when using a word or sentence. In addition, it showed a good (r>0.7) correlation with PTA [11]. Taken together, the DIN test was validated as a way to screen for a hearing loss.

Are the results of DIN test affected when different types of background noise are being presented?

The kind of background noise affected the results of the DIN test. When comparing speech-spectrum noise to multitalker babble noise (MTBN), Ebrahimi, et al. [6] concluded that the speech-spectrum noise showed better (or lower) SRT in the DIN test than did MTBN. In contrast, the interrupted noise with amplitude-modulated characteristics gave the benefit to the DIN test over steady-state noise in the study by Smits, et al. [13], especially for US English. This inconsistency is attributed to complicated effect of both types of noise and the characteristics of the participants. As a simple example, the MTBN had a unique masking feature, called informational masking. In general, noise physically and/or acoustically interferes with speech signals; this is energetic masking. Unlike the energetic masking, informational masking perceptually interrupts the speech signal and is derived from noises such as MTBN [24]. A difference between energetic and informational masking is central processing during the presentation of the speech and noise. MTBN interferes with speech through phonological information and creates more confusion in the central processing than speech-spectrum noise does.

As the participants, people with and without sensorineural hearing loss were participated in the study of Ebrahimi, et al. [6] and Smits, et al. [13] recruited only young adults with normal hearing. Because the patients with sensorineural hearing loss [6], especially at elevated hearing thresholds in the high-frequency range, benefited less from acoustical factors, such as temporal and spectral modulation, the use of noise with frequency- or amplitude-modulation in the DIN test could explain the poor performance for patients with hearing loss [25]. The high frequency hearing loss had a higher correlation with DIN than normal hearing threshold in the presence of interrupted noise rather than broadband noise [14].

What is the most appropriate length of the digits being presented?

It is important to consider the differences among screening tools such as the length of the stimulus. The length of the stimulus could be interpreted as a digit with a monosyllabic or disyllabic structure and single, double and triple digits. Although the International Collegium of Rehabilitative Audiology guideline suggested that the number of syllables in the digit stimulus exclude the effect of phoneme duration which could be affected in a very low SNR condition [26], many studies of the DIN test demonstrated that monosyllabic and bisyllabic digits are less important in terms of perceptual difficulty [9-10,13]. Thus, this study discussed the length of one-, two-, and three-digit stimuli. Miler, et al. [20] confirmed a longer stimulus makes the psychometric function curve shift to the right side (or need higher SNRs). That is, the respondents tended to perform more poorly for longer stimuli than for the short ones.

On the one hand, Smits, et al. [11] agreed that a shorter stimulus could lighten any cognitive burden, such as memory capacity. On the other hand, Versfeld, et al. [27] argued that a long stimulus could increase the efficiency of measurement. They added that supporting a three-digit DIN test produced more reliable and accurate results. In the result of our meta-analysis, triple-digits had the lowest SRTs. However, it was a small difference (effect size: 1.538), and the studies with a triple digit had a large sample size because most studies used it as their stimulus. Wilson, et al.’s [3] comparison reported no noticeable difference between the two- and three-digit stimuli. Therefore, the length of stimulus for the DIN test depended on the testing purpose. When the test was performed for research purpose, the one-digit or two-digit stimulus offered a more accurate process for the measurement and analysis. If the DIN test is conducted for clinical and screening purposes, then the usage of a three-digit stimulus may save time.

Does the patient’s language ability affect the DIN test results?

Based on the subgroup analysis, there was a difference across languages; the DIN test developed with a patient’s mother tongue had lower SRT than did one developed using a second language. As we expected, all studies included in the subgroup analysis supported this result [7,9,13]. However, it is also important to take note of whether the differences across the languages actually occurred due to the language or other factors [7,28]. Zokoll, et al. [28] found meaningful differences in the DIN test for Dutch, German, and Polish, and emphasized consideration of differences in the languages, including spectral and temporal cues for the digits and/or background noise. For example, Smits, et al. [13] compared the results of the DIN test using Dutch and US English. They concluded that the DIN test with US English had a lower SRT than the Dutch one did, even though the subjects were native speakers of Dutch.

From the perspective of acoustical features, the gender of the talker (i.e., a male speaking Dutch and a female speaking US English), the style of speaking (concatenated Dutch and successive US English), and a root mean square level of the materials (-2.36 dB with Dutch and 0.37 dB with US English) were different [13]. These differences in acoustical features could reflect natural differences between the two languages [7,28]. The result of the subgroup analysis in this study revealed a difference across the languages, but this difference could be extrapolated in the internal natures of the languages and their external acoustical features.

In the analysis, DIN tests for native and non-native subjects was compared. Contrary to our prediction, non-native subjects showed a lower SRT on the DIN test [10]. We attribute this difference to the large difference in sample size. Although Potgieter, et al. [10] showed a significant and positive relationship between PTA and SRTs for the DIN test in both native South African English subjects (r=0.76) and non-native English subjects (r=0.69), the asymmetrical distribution of the subjects (i.e., 291 native subjects and 46 non-native subjects) could lead to the overweighted result toward non-native subjects in the determination of effect size calculation. Consequently, the result for non-native subjects was overestimated. Furthermore, the South African English speakers with high English competency accounted for approximately 86% of the participants [10]. In sum, the numbers in American English were already familiar to most people, so it might be possible to test without being overly concerned about the patient’s native language.

Limitations of study and further direction

Several limitations to this study warrant further study. First, although the DIN test is an effective screening tool, other important factors related to presentation and the gender of talkers were not analyzed [7,28]. Second, this study did not compare testing platforms. Although the DIN test can be used on a telephone, smartphone, or tablet, it is necessary to validate each type of platform and to identify whether each platform could provide their designated purpose. Along with the platforms, the effect of types of transducer should be considered. Third, the slope of the DIN test which expressed in the psychometric function curve should be analyzed and discussed to confirm the reliability and validity of the DIN test. Finally, the results of a meta-analysis showed a high level of heterogeneity although each study still had high quality. In other words, the absence of systematic methodology, such as acoustical features (i.e., differences between genders, style of speaking, and the intensity level of material presentation) and the optimization process (i.e., composition of the digits and background noise) led to the divergent results. In the future, a large-scale study with a specific and unified methodology should be conducted to minimize inconsistent results and confirm the reliability and validity of the DIN test.

In sum, the DIN test was developed for several languages and has been evaluated using a well-designed and systematic process. The components of the test including stimuli and background noise were also tried. A more elaborate procedure, such as an optimization process, would produce a clear comparison across languages and confirm the value of the DIN test for hearing screening in a variety of settings.

Supplementary Materials

The online-only Data Supplement is available with this article at https://doi.org/10.7874/jao.2021.00416.

Supplementary Fig. 1.

The developmental history of various languages for the digit-in-noise test over two decades.

jao-2021-00416-suppl.pdf

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korea government (MSIT) (2019R1F1A1053060).

Notes

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no financial conflicts of interest.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: all authors. Data curation: Chanbeom Kwak, Woojae Han. Formal analysis: Chanbeom Kwak, Woojae Han. Funding acquisition: Woojae Han. Methodology: Chanbeom Kwak, Yonghee Oh. Project administration: Jae-Hyun Seo, Woojae Han. Resources: Jae-Hyun Seo, Woojae Han. Software: Chanbeom Kwak. Supervision: Jae-Hyun Seo, Yonghee Oh, Woojae Han. Validation: all authors. Visualization: Jae-Hyun Seo, Yonghee Oh. Writing—original draft: Chanbeom Kwak. Writing—review & editing: all authors. Approval of final manuscript: all authors.

References

1. Yueh B, Shapiro N, MacLean CH, Shekelle PG. Screening and management of adult hearing loss in primary care: scientific review. JAMA 2003;289:1976–85.
2. Samelli AG, Rabelo CM, Sanches SGG, Aquino CP. Tablet-based hearing screening test. Telemed J E Health 2017;23:747–52.
3. Wilson RH, Burks CA, Weakley DG. A comparison of word-recognition abilities assessed with digit pairs and digit triplets in multitalker babble. J Rehabil Res Dev 2005;42:499–510.
4. Denys S, Hofmann M, van Wieringen A, Wouters J. Improving the efficiency of the digit triplet test using digit scoring with variable adaptive step sizes. Int J Audiol 2019;58:670–7.
5. Dillon H, Beach EF, Seymour J, Carter L, Golding M. Development of Telscreen: a telephone-based speech-in-noise hearing screening test with a novel masking noise and scoring procedure. Int J Audiol 2016;55:463–71.
6. Ebrahimi A, Mahdavi ME, Jalilvand H. Auditory recognition of Persian digits in presence of speech-spectrum noise and multi-talker babble: a validation study. Aud Vestib Res 2020;29:39–47.
7. Giguère C, Lagacé J, Ellaham NN, Pichora-Fuller MK, Goy H, Bégin C, et al. Development of the Canadian digit triplet test in English and French. J Acoust Soc Am 2020;147:EL252–8.
8. Jansen S, Luts H, Wagener KC, Frachet B, Wouters J. The French digit triplet test: a hearing screening tool for speech intelligibility in noise. Int J Audiol 2010;49:378–87.
9. Ozimek E, Kutzner D, Sęk A, Wicher A. Development and evaluation of Polish digit triplet test for auditory screening. Speech Commun 2009;51:307–16.
10. Potgieter JM, Swanepoel DW, Myburgh HC, Smits C. The South African English smartphone digits-in-noise hearing test: effect of age, hearing loss, and speaking competence. Ear Hear 2018;39:656–63.
11. Smits C, Kapteyn TS, Houtgast T. Development and validation of an automatic speech-in-noise screening test by telephone. Int J Audiol 2004;43:15–28.
12. Smits C, Theo Goverts S, Festen JM. The digits-in-noise test: assessing auditory speech recognition abilities in noise. J Acoust Soc Am 2013;133:1693–706.
13. Smits C, Watson CS, Kidd GR, Moore DR, Goverts ST. A comparison between the Dutch and American-English digits-in-noise (DIN) tests in normal-hearing listeners. Int J Audiol 2016;55:358–65.
14. Vlaming MS, MacKinnon RC, Jansen M, Moore DR. Automated screening for high-frequency hearing loss. Ear Hear 2014;35:667–79.
15. Willberg T, Buschermöhle M, Sivonen V, Aarnisalo AA, Löppönen H, Kollmeier B, et al. The development and evaluation of the Finnish digit triplet test. Acta Otolaryngol 2016;136:1035–40.
16. Wilson RH, Weakley DG. The use of digit triplets to evaluate word-recognition abilities in multitalker babble. Semin Hear 2004;25:93–111.
17. Watson CS, Kidd GR, Miller JD, Smits C, Humes LE. Telephone screening tests for functionally impaired hearing: current use in seven countries and development of a US version. J Am Acad Audiol 2012;23:757–67.
18. Moher D, Shamseer L, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst Rev 2015;4:1–9.
19. Moher D, Chersi D, Dooley G, Stewart L, Clarke M, Macleod M. PROSPERO: International prospective register of systematic reviews [Internet]. York: National Institute for Health Research; [cited 2020 Dec 2]. Available from: http://cdn.elsevier.com/promis_misc/PROSPEROAnimal.pdf.
20. Miller GA, Heise GA, Lichten W. The intelligibility of speech as a function of the context of the test materials. J Exp Psychol 1951;41:329–35.
21. Blobaum P. Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro). J Med Libr Assoc 2006;94:477–8.
22. Moseley AM, Elkins MR, Van der Wees PJ, Pinheiro MB. Using research to guide practice: the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro). Braz J Phys Ther 2020;24:384–91.
23. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analysis. BMJ 2003;327:557–60.
24. Lidestam B, Holgersson J, Moradi S. Comparison of informational vs. energetic masking effects on speechreading performance. Front Psychol 2014;5:639.
25. Paraouty N, Ewert SD, Wallaert N, Lorenzi C. Interactions between amplitude modulation and frequency modulation processing: effects of age and hearing loss. J Acoust Soc Am 2016;140:121–31.
26. Akeroyd MA, Arlinger S, Bentler RA, Boothroyd A, Dillier N, Dreschler WA, et al. International Collegium of Rehabilitative Audiology (ICRA) recommendations for the construction of multilingual speech tests: ICRA Working Group on multilingual speech tests. Int J Audiol 2015;54:17–22.
27. Versfeld NJ, Daalder L, Festen JM, Houtgast T. Method for the selection of sentence materials for efficient measurement of the speech reception threshold. J Acoust Soc Am 2000;107:1671–84.
28. Zokoll MA, Wagener KC, Brand T, Buschermöhle M, Kollmeier B. Internationally comparable screening tests for listening in noise in several European languages: the German digit triplet test as an optimization prototype. Int J Audiol 2012;51:697–707.

Article information Continued

Fig. 1.

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) flow diagram that visually expresses the inclusion and exclusion process of the current study. PICOS, participants, intervention, control, outcome measures, and study design.

Fig. 2.

Forest plot (A) and funnel plot (B) of the 14 reviewed studies analyzed using standardized mean differences.

Fig. 3.

The forest plot for the subgroup analysis by hearing condition (A), types of stimulus (B) and noise (C), kinds of language (D), and language competence (E).

Table 1.

Inclusion criteria for the current study based on participants, intervention, control, outcomes, and study designs (PICOS)

PICOS Content
Participants Adults 18 years or older with and without hearing loss except for the use of any kind of hearing assistive device (i.e., hearing aids, cochlear implant)
Intervention Digit-in-noise test using various languages and such stimuli as single digit, digit pair, and digit triplet
Control Comparison to a control group or repeated measures (experiments with additional purposes)
Outcomes Outcome measure(s) related to development, reliability, efficacy, and/or standardization of a digit-in-noise test (i.e., a comparison of types of stimuli, different hearing threshold groups, and between languages)
Study design Randomized controlled trials, non-randomized controlled trials, between-group comparisons, and repeated measures (experiments with additional purposes)

Table 2.

Analysis using the scientific study validity criteria based on PEDro checklists

Study Item
Total Study quality
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Wilson, et al. [3] 1 0 1 1 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 8/11 Good
Denys, et al. [4] 1 0 1 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 7/11 Good
Ebrahimi, et al. [6] 0 1 1 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 7/11 Good
Giguère, et al. [7] 1 0 0 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 6/11 Good
Jansen, et al. [8] 1 0 0 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 6/11 Good
Potgieter, et al. [10] 1 0 1 1 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 8/11 Good
Smits, et al. [11] 1 0 1 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 7/11 Good
Smits, et al. [12] 1 0 1 1 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 8/11 Good
Smits, et al. [13] 0 0 1 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 6/11 Good
Vlaming, et al. [14] 1 0 1 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 7/11 Good
Willberg, et al. [15] 1 0 0 1 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 7/11 Good
Wilson and Weakely [16] 1 0 1 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 7/11 Good
Dillon, et al. [5] 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 5/11 Fair
Ozimek, et al. [9] 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 4/11 Fair

1 and 0 stand for “Yes” and “No,” respectively. The Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro) Scale consisted of 11 items as follows: 1) eligibility criteria were specified; 2) subjects were randomly allocated to groups; 3) allocation was concealed; 4) the groups were similar at baseline regarding the most important prognostic indicators; 5) there was a blinding of all subjects; 6) there was a blinding of all therapists who administered the therapy; 7) there was a blinding of all assessors who measured at least one key outcome; 8) measures for at least one key outcome were obtained from more than 85% of the subjects who were initially allocated to groups; 9) all subjects for whom outcome measures were available received the treatment or control condition as allocated or, where this was not the case, the data for at least one key outcome was analyzed using the intention to treat; 10) the results of between-group statistical comparisons reported for at least one key outcome; 11) the study providing both point measures and measures of variability for at least one key outcome

Table 3.

Characteristics and main findings for all enrolled studies for the participants, the intervention, control group, and the outcome of each study

Study Participants Intervention Control group Study design Outcome measures Main findings
Wilson, et al. [3] Thirty-two older patients ages 46 to 85 years with sensorineural hearing loss Digit pair and digit triplet with background noise multi-talker babble were used as stimuli. Digits 1 through 10 (excluding 7) were mixed with various levels of multi-talker babble noise (i.e., 4 to -20 dB with a 4 dB step). Sixteen young adults ages 20 to 29 years with NH Comparison to the control group and the repeated measures Comparison between hearing groups and types of stimuli, SNR Repeated-measures of ANOVAs indicated that the differences between the data for the digit pairs and the digit triplets were significant (listeners with NH: [F(1,15)=32.609, p≤0.001]; patients with hearing loss: [F(1,31)=19.633, p<0.001].
Denys, et al. [4] The 45 adults ages 18 to 71 years with hearing impaired Twenty-one digit triplets were presented with various conditions, such as T79, T79LP, D79, D57, and D35. The T and D stand for triplet and digit, respectively. The numbers, such as 79, 57, and 35, represent the recognition probabilities. Twenty-three adults ages 17 to 61 years with NH Comparison to a control group and repeated measures Comparison between hearing groups, SRT All correlations between PTA and SRT were significant when tested for all participants. and only HI participants, but none for only NH participants (p<0.001 and p>0.05, respectively).
Dillon, et al. [5] Seventy-five adults age 25 to 86 years with and without hearing loss Each of the 81digit triplets were called to varying RMS levels and then mixed with the new masking noise to create files at 18 SNR levels’ ranging from -28 to +4 SNR. None Repeated measures Comparison between types of noise, SRTn A significant relationship was evident between experiment 2 results and the 4-frequency averaged hearing level (r=0.77, p<0.001).
Ebrahimi, et al. [6] Nineteen adults (mean age: 50.7 years) with sensorineural hearing loss Persian monosyllabic digits of 1 to 10 were extracted from Farsi Auditory Recognition of Digit-in-Noise (FARDIN) test. Based on the digit stimuli, the Persian speech-shaped noise was created. The multi-talker babble noise contained the voices of six talkers. Twenty adults (mean age: 23.4 years) with NH Comparison to a control group and repeated measures Comparison between hearing groups and types of noise, SNR Mean correct recognition score in the presence of speech-shaped noise in the hearing loss group was significantly weaker than that of the NH group (p<0.05 for digits 1, 2, 5, 7, 8, 10, and p<0.001 for digits 3, 6, and 8). However, the mean correct recognition of digits 1, 2, 9, and 10 in the presence of multi-talker babble noise did not show a significant difference between NH and hearing loss groups.
Giguère, et al. [7] A total of 112 young adults ages 18 to 30 years with NH. Subjects were accounted for 2 language groups (English and French). Eight monosyllabic digits (0 to 9, except for 0 and 7) recorded by four talkers (English-male, English-female, French-male, and French-female) were manipulated into digit triplet. A unique speech-shaped noise matching the long-term average spectrum of the digit materials was used. None Repeated measures Comparison between language groups, SRTn A repeated measures one-way analysis of variance was conducted on the adaptive data for each language-talker version of the test, with SRT as the outcome measure and List as a within-subject factor. There was no effect of List on SRT for the English male [F(3,45)=0.17, p=0.92], French male [F(3,45)=1.46, p=0.24], and French female [F(3,45)=0.29, p=0.83] talker versions, respectively. However, there was an effect of List on SRT for the English-female talker [F(3,45)=3.76, p=0.017].
Jansen, et al. [8] A total of 40 ears participated. The 19 out of the 40 ears were NH and the remaining 21 ears were HI. The French digit triplet with -12 to +8 dB SNR range of 2 dB steps. The 19 ears with NH Repeated measures Comparison between hearing groups, SRT The correlation between SRT scores and PTA of 4-frequencies average (0.5, 1, 2, 4 kHz) was 0.77 and proved to be significant (p<0.001).
Ozimek, et al. [9] Fifty adults with NH (22 female and 28 male) Polish digit triplet that confirmed the developed and evaluated process were used. A total of 100 digit triplets were divided into 4 lists containing 25 different triplets each. None Repeated measures Comparison between languages, SRT The mean SRT and SRT50mean characterizing each list fell into the range of ±0.1 dB and ±1%/dB, respectively, of the mean SRT and mean S50 for the 100 selected triplets, i.e., -9.4 dB and 21.4%/dB, respectively, (i.e., the lists are composed of different triplets, but yielding similar intelligibility).
Potgieter, et al. [10] A total of 458 adults ages 16 to 90 years with and without hearing loss. All subjects were divided into various group based on the multiple purpose, such as English competence score. Digit triplet lists from the South African English smartphone-based digits-in-noise test (0 to 9, 10 digits) were used. The 337 out of 458 adults who had NH were from 16 to 81 years in age. Comparison to a control group and the repeated measures Comparison between native and non-native speaker, SRT The average normal-hearing SRT in the native and non-native ≥6 group was approximately 1.7 dB lower (better) than in the non-native ≤5 group.
Smits, et al. [11] The total group of 76 ears included two ears with pure conductive loss and 7 ears with mixed hearing loss. The remaining 67 ears consisted of normal-hearing ears or ears with perceptive hearing loss. Dutch digits from 0 to 9 (except for 7 and 9) were manipulated into digit triplets. None Repeated measures Comparison between hearing groups and types of transducer, SRTn The highest correlation (r=0.866) between SRTn measurements was found between the newly developed test (triplet SRTn test by telephone) and the reference test (sentence SRTn test by headphones).
Smits, et al. [12] A total of forty adults ages 18 to 25 years with NH The digit triplet material from the Dutch version of the DIN test. To identify the effect of hearing loss on the DIN test, six simulated hearing loss conditions (unprocessed, low-pass filtered at 3 kHz, smeared, smeared and low-pass filtered at 3 kHz, smeared and low-pass filtered at 1 kHz, and smeared and low-pass filtered at 0.5 kHz) were used. Modified version of Dutch (from the NL DIN) and None Repeated measures Comparison between hearing groups, SRT The intelligibility of the smeared digit-triplet speech material was relatively insensitive to low-pass filtering; the average intelligibility was still 79% at a LP cut-off frequency of 250 Hz.
Smits, et al. [13] Sixteen adults ages 19 to 25 years (14 female, 2 male) with NH American-English (from the US DIN) digit triplets were used. While the NL DIN contained 10 digits (0 to 9) and a recorded male speaker, the US DIN consisted of only 8 digits (1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, 9) and a recorded female voice. None Repeated measures A comparison between languages and types of noise, SRT Separate repeated measures of ANOVA intended to identify the effects of types of noise and revealed that there was a significant main effect for condition [F(3,45)=818.31, p<0.001]. Neither the main effect for the DIN test version nor the interaction between condition and the DIN test version was significant.
Vlaming, et al. [14] Fifty adults ages 31 to 75 years with impaired hearing The digits from 0 to 9 (excluding 7) were recorded as triplets. 24 adults ages 18 to 47 years with NH Comparison to a control group and repeated measures Comparison between hearing groups and types of noise, SRT For a reference of test performance of the new HF tests, the SRTs of the NH group were analyzed. Mean SRTs of -21.3 dB (HF-triplets; SD=2.4 dB) and -21.1 dB (HF-CVC; SD=2.1 dB) were found. For the broadband digit-triplet test, the mean SRT was -10.3 dB (SD=1.1 dB).
Willberg, et al. [15] Nineteen native Finnish speakers age 18 to 34 years with NH The speech material of the Finnish digit triplet test consisted of the digits from 0 to 9 combined into triplets. None Repeated measures Comparison between types of stimuli, SRT Averaged across all test subjects, the mean SRT was -10.8±0.5 dB SNR when triplet scoring was used. With triplet scoring. the mean slope of the reference function of the Finnish digit triplet test was 23.4±5.2%/dB.
Wilson and Weakely [16] Forty-eight older patients (mean age: 63.5 years) with sensorineural hearing loss The nine digits (1 to 10, except for 7) with 14 SNR conditions (quiet and 14 SNR conditions from 6 to -20 dB, 2dB steps) were used. As background noise, multi-talker babble noise was used. 24 young adults (mean age=20.6 years) with NH Comparison to a control group and repeated measures Comparison between hearing groups, 50% correct points There was no significant difference between the trials for either group of patients F(1,46)=0.811, p=0.37] for the listeners with NH, and for the patients with hearing loss [F(1,94)=0.128, p=0.72].

NL, Netherlands; US, United States; DIN, digit-in-noise test; SRT, speech recognition threshold; SRTn, speech recognition threshold with a digit; SNR, signal-to-noise ratio; RMS, rootmean square; LP, low-pass filter; NH, normal hearing; HI, hearing-impaired; PTA, pure-tone average; HF, high-frequency; CVC, consonant-vowel-consonant; SD, standard deviation